by Edward QuinnMacKinnon and Fuentes (2012) draw on the literature of primatology to place human sociality in a broader phylogenetic context. This context allows us to understand humans as part of a pattern in evolutionary history, which associates increased sociality with increasingly large brains. Placing ourselves within larger evolutionary patterns also allows us to see how our social behavior might be different from that of non-human primates. What is clear in this chapter is that behavioral flexibility has allowed many different primates to exploit myriad environments in many different ways. MacKinnon and Fuentes (2012) use the example of variation in food processing within the same species in the same type of environments to illustrate this point. This variation must be understood not only in terms of ecology, but also in terms of the social traditions of particular groups within species.
The authors illustrate the use of theories and methods like niche construction and social network analysis for a better understanding of how social and ecological selective forces are integrated in the production of the social primate brain, and how this brain enhances selective fitness. The particularly long period of post-natal brain growth in genera like Homo, Pan, and Cebus allows for the deep integration of social learning into the developing brain. This period of development is crucial for learning how to navigate (social) selective landscapes successfully, which call simultaneously for competition and cooperation with variable actors. The authors ultimately agree with scholars like Dunbar, arguing that social intelligence (enabled by expanded brain volume and longer periods of postnatal brain development) was a major determinant of darwinian success in human evolution. I found the niche construction section of this reading useful. Rather than conceiving of social competitors, foraging problems, predator avoidance, etc. as independent selective pressures calling for specific adaptive strategies on the part of the individual, it is useful to think about how social behavior and ecology are integrated. Niche construction theory helps to provide a plausible account of how human brain evolution could have occurred; however, I am left wondering if the social brain hypothesis isn’t another “just-so” story. It is certainly conceivable that larger brains enabled more cooperation, increasingly complex communication, more efficient exploitation of the environment, a greater range of exploitable habitats, etc., but how could we disprove this hypothesis? I might be more convinced by the social brain hypothesis if there were neurological models of how the social brain could have developed through evolutionary time. What specific changes occurred in brain structure and circuitry in human evolutionary history that allowed for the development of such a uniquely social brain? It is necessary to talk about changes in brain volume, but this only part of the story. I think this is the sort of perspective a neuroanthropologist might contribute to primate studies of cognition and the social brain hypothesis. I am admittedly ignorant when it comes to methodology in primate studies, but I assume primate studies work with small samples, often in an artificial laboratory environment, with evidence from the field sometimes taking the form of anecdotes. I was therefore impressed with some of the literature cited having to do with social network analysis. In particular, I was impressed by the very large amount of data being integrated into the study of primate social networks related to kinship, particular behaviors such as grooming, and how these different networks interact to produce population level patterns in social behavior. This reading left me with an improved impression of primatology studies. This chapter clearly demonstrates the utility of theory and method in primatology for a better understanding of humans. It helps us answer big questions: why are humans so social? Where does our capacity for culture come from? Thus, taking the brain as a given in neuroanthropological research would be a mistake. More productive research will no doubt conceive of the brain as a product of specific evolutionary processes, which impact on how our brain develops and functions in everyday settings. This is a tangible contribution of primatology to neuroanthropology, and this area of research deserves further investigation. References MacKinnon, Katherine C., and Agustín Fuentes 2012 Primate Social Cognition, Human Evolution, and Niche Construction: A Core Context for Neuroanthropology. In The Encultured Brain: An Introduction to Neuroanthropology. Daniel H. Lende and Greg Downey, eds. Pp. 67-102. Cambridge: The MIT Press.
11 Comments
Danielle Long
1/26/2016 03:20:16 pm
I agree with you about the niche construction theory because it is a different outlook on how it can prove the evolution of our society (like the example was used in the book- we needed tools to make things more sufficient and grow and survive in the environment). But it just could be another theory because we can't get physical proof or test the theory at the start of human evolution by monitoring the brain and thought process.
Reply
Larry Monocello
1/27/2016 09:29:08 am
On the topic of niche construction, I think that it's an interesting hypothesis. As with all adaptationist, hypotheses, however, it is important to avoid "just-so" stories, as Edward rightly pointed out. The use of social network analysis also then kind of brings MacKinnon and Fuentes together with Dunbar and Schultz in positing that interpersonal relationships and their computational complexity led to the expansion of the brain.
Reply
Larry Monocello (2nd)
2/23/2016 07:09:34 pm
Reconsideration of these articles made me wonder about the degree to which the culturally mediated types and degrees of social interaction affect the brain, if at all. Surely, interpersonal interactions differ cross-culturally, cross-class, and even cross-age group.
Reply
Olivia Davis
1/27/2016 10:02:24 am
Throughout the MacKinnon and Fuentes reading, I found myself stumbling over the cliche saying, "which came first, the chicken or the egg?" in regards to a lot of the theories about primate social cognition that were addressed. Now, an explicit answer to this question may not actually exist but, for the sake of discussion, I'd like to think that an individual's initial adaption to an environment came before the adaption to the social group to which the individual belongs. Some evidence to support my claim lies in the literature on social groups and the way that child-rearing alters an individual both biologically and socially (I.e. central nervous system, digestion, interaction, response to social stimuli, etc.).
Reply
Olivia Davis
3/2/2016 08:35:52 am
Although I found this chapter interesting, our class discussions (to the best of my memory) didn't exactly touch on this idea of sociality as an adaptation to a social environment. I still believe that one's ability to perform socially is a response to the environment in which an individual is raised and could possibly be viewed and studied as the primary adaptation postnatally speaking.
Reply
Molly Jaworski
1/27/2016 10:15:33 am
In reading this chapter I found myself connecting information to courses I have previously taken. 'Niche' is a term I have become extremely familiar with from ecology, biology, and physical anthropology courses and is an important factor in sustaining life. A niche describes the relation between a species or population in an ecosystem. Species and ecosystem relationship are key in understanding ecological niches. In the reading Downey and Lende expand on the term niche, taking it to another level and discussing what is called a 'niche construction' hypothesis. In this aspect they take this idea of niche construction and apply it in a social context, determining that they interrelate with one another. Through niche construction they determine that social living and interactions are contributing factors in determining a species' niche in an ecosystem.
Reply
Molly Jaworski
1/27/2016 11:09:31 am
I made a mistake and need to correct credit to be given to MacKinnon and Fuentes not Downey and Lende for this reading.
Reply
Molly Jaworski
3/2/2016 07:41:21 am
After further discussion and rereading I find myself a little curious as to how culture and cross cultural interactions influence this niche construction. Downey and Lende state that social living and interactions contribute to the determination of a species' niche, but human social living and interactions vary across cultures. So is it safe to say that though niche construction is generally species specific, it can also have subfields or niches that describe the vairations in cultures for humans?
Issac
1/27/2016 02:24:33 pm
What I find most interesting about the niche construction hypothesis is the fact that it is not a one way street. What I mean is that through constructing niches we influence our environment, which in turn influences us. A key point of the definition of niche construction is the fact of actively changing a factor of the environment, but it does not end there. Not only do we create niches that impact the enviroment , but by doing this we also can alter the environmental pressures that later impact ourselves and our children. A sentence from this chapter may be able to summarize this idea better than I can where it simply says, " organisms have significant effects on their environment, which can then affect those local populations".
Reply
April Irwin
3/2/2016 01:32:49 pm
Although I am not terribly interested in primatology, despite knowing that it has helped us so much with understanding human behavior, our discussion about niche construction was really helpful with showing me that primatology does have an impact on how we study humans. Niche construction relates directly to the social foundations of formal education because formal education is the systematic method of teaching children how to form socially acceptable relationships with their group, population, and environment. In terms of my research, I think framing education as a form of niche construction will be helpful in bridging a gap between education research and anthropology research.
Reply
12/27/2021 01:52:11 am
I found this article very interesting, thanks for sharing
Reply
Leave a Reply. |
AuthorThis blog is group authored by Dr. DeCaro and the students in his ANT 474/574: Neuroanthropology. Archives
April 2019
Categories |